In Mandarin two-character compound words, the syllable of the second rightmost constituent may be neutralized, which in combination with the first full tone as to form a neutral tone compound with a trochee prosody pattern [Strong-weak]. Because a constituent (character in writing form) can carry more than one tone, it is possible that a constituent has both a full tone form and a neutral tone form, which may be semantically different. This study focused on the prosodic features of the second constituent in both disyllabic neutral tone and full tone compounds across four full tones contexts. Our goal was to investigate the acoustic features of neutral tones in contrast with their full tone counterparts. For this purpose, we selected 80 minimal pairs of two-character compounds, i.e., 巴掌 /ba1zhang0/ (clap) and 手掌 /shou3zhang3/ (hands), their second constituents present the identical syllabic structure /zhang/ and the contrastive tone patterns (in this example, neutral tone (Tone 0) vs. full tone (Tone 3)). One-hundred sixty target compound words were embedded within a short carrier sentence and recorded by native speakers of Mandarin. The acoustic correlates of the paired second syllables within the minimal pairs were examined by using Praat and R-studio, including the duration, pitch (F0) and intensity, and the averaged pitch curve of target compounds are represented through the point-to-point graphical comparisons. Our results indicate two main findings. Firstly, we confirmed that the acoustic features of neutral tone are phonetically reduced in contrast to the full tone. Secondly, neutral tone shares some common features with their full tone counterparts in the intensity and pitch level, whereas the duration and pitch range reveal barely any significant differences across four full tones contexts. In the results, the pitch onset value of the second neutral tone is mainly dependent on the offset of the preceding full tone, except for the Tone 4. Our results suggest a phonetic interaction between the first full tone constituent and second neutral tone constituent, which lends support to the assumption that neutral tone is phonetically reduced in comparison with full tones, and it can be predicted on the basis of acoustic features of the first constituent like pitch offset.
Published in | International Journal of Language and Linguistics (Volume 11, Issue 4) |
DOI | 10.11648/j.ijll.20231104.13 |
Page(s) | 107-118 |
Creative Commons |
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited. |
Copyright |
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Science Publishing Group |
Compounds, Mandarin, Neutral Tone, Full Tone
[1] | Yip, M. J., Tone. 2002: Cambridge University Press. |
[2] | Duanmu, S., The phonology of standard Chinese. 2007: OUP Oxford. |
[3] | Shen, X. S., Mandarin neutral tone revisited. Acta linguistica hafniensia, 1992. 24 (1): p. 131-152. |
[4] | Li, C. N. and S. A. Thompson, Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference grammar. Vol. 3. 1989: Univ of California Press. |
[5] | Hua, Z. and B. Dodd, The phonological acquisition of Putonghua (modern standard Chinese). Journal of child language, 2000. 27 (1): p. 3-42. |
[6] | Zhou, X. and W. Marslen-Wilson, Words, morphemes and syllables in the Chinese mental lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1994. 9 (3): p. 393-422. |
[7] | Liu, S. and A. G. Samuel, Perception of Mandarin lexical tones when F0 information is neutralized. Language and speech, 2004. 47 (2): p. 109-138. |
[8] | Gandour, J., Tone perception in Far Eastern languages. Journal of phonetics, 1983. 11 (2): p. 149-175. |
[9] | Lin, M. and J. Yan, Neutral tone and stress in Mandarin Chinese. Language Teaching and Linguistic Studies, 1990. 3: p. 88-104. |
[10] | Lee, W.-S. and E. Zee, Prosodic characteristics of the neutral tone in Beijing Mandarin. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 2008. 36 (1): p. 1-29. |
[11] | Tang, P., A study of prosodic errors of Chinese neutral tone by advanced Japanese students (Chinese version). TCSOL Studies, 2014. 56 (4): p. 39-47. |
[12] | Wang, Y., The effects of pitch and duration on the perception of the neutral tone in standard Chinese. Acta Acustica, 2004. |
[13] | Wang, J., The representation of the neutral tone in Chinese Putonghua. Studies in Chinese phonology, 1997. 20: p. 157. |
[14] | Wai-Sum, L. and Z. Eric, Chinese phonetics, in The Handbook of Chinese Linguistics. 2014, Wiley-Blackwell. p. 369-399. |
[15] | Chao, Y. R., Language and symbolic systems. Vol. 260. 1968: Cambridge University Press Cambridge. |
[16] | Sproat, R. and C. Shih. Prenominal adjectival ordering in English and Mandarin. in North East Linguistics Society. 1987. |
[17] | Gao, Y.-z., Bëijïng huà de qïngshëng wènti, [The neutral tone in the Beijing dialect]. Yûyân Jiàoxué yü Yanjîu, 1980. 2: p. 82-98. |
[18] | Lin, C., Typologies sinographiques et incidences didactique. 2007, Lyon 3. |
[19] | Cao, J., A study on neutral tone in Standard Chinese. Appl. Acoust, 1986. 4: p. 1-6. |
[20] | Yip, M. J., The tonal phonology of Chinese. 1980, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. |
[21] | Li, Z., The phonetics and phonology of tone mapping in a constraint-based approach. 2003, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. |
[22] | Sproat, R. W., Multilingual text-to-speech synthesis. 1997: KLUWER academic publishers. |
[23] | Chen, Y. and Y. Xu, Production of weak elements in speech–evidence from f0 patterns of neutral tone in Standard Chinese. Phonetica, 2006. 63 (1): p. 47-75. |
[24] | Zhao, G., The contemporary Chinese dictionary. 2015, Oxford University Press. |
[25] | Boersma, P. Praat: doing phonetics by computer. 2006; Available from: http://www.praat.org/. |
[26] | Xu, Y., Contextual tonal variations in Mandarin. Journal of phonetics, 1997. 25 (1): p. 61-83. |
[27] | Yang, S., The synthetic rules for neutral tone in standard Chinese. J. Applied Acoustics, 1991. 10 (1): p. 12-18. |
[28] | Shen, X. S., Relative duration as a perceptual cue to stress in Mandarin. Language and Speech, 1993. 36 (4): p. 415-433. |
APA Style
Zhongpei Zhang, Anne Lacheret-Dujour, Frédéric Isel. (2023). Phonetic Correlates of Lexical Neural Tone Within Mandarin Two-Character Compounds in Four Full Tones Contexts. International Journal of Language and Linguistics, 11(4), 107-118. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20231104.13
ACS Style
Zhongpei Zhang; Anne Lacheret-Dujour; Frédéric Isel. Phonetic Correlates of Lexical Neural Tone Within Mandarin Two-Character Compounds in Four Full Tones Contexts. Int. J. Lang. Linguist. 2023, 11(4), 107-118. doi: 10.11648/j.ijll.20231104.13
AMA Style
Zhongpei Zhang, Anne Lacheret-Dujour, Frédéric Isel. Phonetic Correlates of Lexical Neural Tone Within Mandarin Two-Character Compounds in Four Full Tones Contexts. Int J Lang Linguist. 2023;11(4):107-118. doi: 10.11648/j.ijll.20231104.13
@article{10.11648/j.ijll.20231104.13, author = {Zhongpei Zhang and Anne Lacheret-Dujour and Frédéric Isel}, title = {Phonetic Correlates of Lexical Neural Tone Within Mandarin Two-Character Compounds in Four Full Tones Contexts}, journal = {International Journal of Language and Linguistics}, volume = {11}, number = {4}, pages = {107-118}, doi = {10.11648/j.ijll.20231104.13}, url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20231104.13}, eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.ijll.20231104.13}, abstract = {In Mandarin two-character compound words, the syllable of the second rightmost constituent may be neutralized, which in combination with the first full tone as to form a neutral tone compound with a trochee prosody pattern [Strong-weak]. Because a constituent (character in writing form) can carry more than one tone, it is possible that a constituent has both a full tone form and a neutral tone form, which may be semantically different. This study focused on the prosodic features of the second constituent in both disyllabic neutral tone and full tone compounds across four full tones contexts. Our goal was to investigate the acoustic features of neutral tones in contrast with their full tone counterparts. For this purpose, we selected 80 minimal pairs of two-character compounds, i.e., 巴掌 /ba1zhang0/ (clap) and 手掌 /shou3zhang3/ (hands), their second constituents present the identical syllabic structure /zhang/ and the contrastive tone patterns (in this example, neutral tone (Tone 0) vs. full tone (Tone 3)). One-hundred sixty target compound words were embedded within a short carrier sentence and recorded by native speakers of Mandarin. The acoustic correlates of the paired second syllables within the minimal pairs were examined by using Praat and R-studio, including the duration, pitch (F0) and intensity, and the averaged pitch curve of target compounds are represented through the point-to-point graphical comparisons. Our results indicate two main findings. Firstly, we confirmed that the acoustic features of neutral tone are phonetically reduced in contrast to the full tone. Secondly, neutral tone shares some common features with their full tone counterparts in the intensity and pitch level, whereas the duration and pitch range reveal barely any significant differences across four full tones contexts. In the results, the pitch onset value of the second neutral tone is mainly dependent on the offset of the preceding full tone, except for the Tone 4. Our results suggest a phonetic interaction between the first full tone constituent and second neutral tone constituent, which lends support to the assumption that neutral tone is phonetically reduced in comparison with full tones, and it can be predicted on the basis of acoustic features of the first constituent like pitch offset.}, year = {2023} }
TY - JOUR T1 - Phonetic Correlates of Lexical Neural Tone Within Mandarin Two-Character Compounds in Four Full Tones Contexts AU - Zhongpei Zhang AU - Anne Lacheret-Dujour AU - Frédéric Isel Y1 - 2023/07/24 PY - 2023 N1 - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20231104.13 DO - 10.11648/j.ijll.20231104.13 T2 - International Journal of Language and Linguistics JF - International Journal of Language and Linguistics JO - International Journal of Language and Linguistics SP - 107 EP - 118 PB - Science Publishing Group SN - 2330-0221 UR - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ijll.20231104.13 AB - In Mandarin two-character compound words, the syllable of the second rightmost constituent may be neutralized, which in combination with the first full tone as to form a neutral tone compound with a trochee prosody pattern [Strong-weak]. Because a constituent (character in writing form) can carry more than one tone, it is possible that a constituent has both a full tone form and a neutral tone form, which may be semantically different. This study focused on the prosodic features of the second constituent in both disyllabic neutral tone and full tone compounds across four full tones contexts. Our goal was to investigate the acoustic features of neutral tones in contrast with their full tone counterparts. For this purpose, we selected 80 minimal pairs of two-character compounds, i.e., 巴掌 /ba1zhang0/ (clap) and 手掌 /shou3zhang3/ (hands), their second constituents present the identical syllabic structure /zhang/ and the contrastive tone patterns (in this example, neutral tone (Tone 0) vs. full tone (Tone 3)). One-hundred sixty target compound words were embedded within a short carrier sentence and recorded by native speakers of Mandarin. The acoustic correlates of the paired second syllables within the minimal pairs were examined by using Praat and R-studio, including the duration, pitch (F0) and intensity, and the averaged pitch curve of target compounds are represented through the point-to-point graphical comparisons. Our results indicate two main findings. Firstly, we confirmed that the acoustic features of neutral tone are phonetically reduced in contrast to the full tone. Secondly, neutral tone shares some common features with their full tone counterparts in the intensity and pitch level, whereas the duration and pitch range reveal barely any significant differences across four full tones contexts. In the results, the pitch onset value of the second neutral tone is mainly dependent on the offset of the preceding full tone, except for the Tone 4. Our results suggest a phonetic interaction between the first full tone constituent and second neutral tone constituent, which lends support to the assumption that neutral tone is phonetically reduced in comparison with full tones, and it can be predicted on the basis of acoustic features of the first constituent like pitch offset. VL - 11 IS - 4 ER -